Da Musically Inclined Bomb

DePauw University's First Year Seminar on Writing about Music

Monday, December 04, 2006

is that pig over there?

Learning Absolute Pitch by Children: A Cross Sectional Study

Hello. I am here today to discuss the article, Learning Absolute Pitch by Children: A Cross Sectional Study by Ken’ichi Miyazaki & Yoko Ogawa.

Fist off, I thought I’d start by letting you know about some key words I will be using throughout my presentation. Absolute Pitch or AP is the ability to recognize or sing a given isolated note, and it is also called “perfect pitch. Early learning is the view that AP is learned by extensive training or repeated exposure to musical stimuli. Critical period is the time during early childhood when AP develops, and after this critical period training cannot produce AP. Pitch naming is producing the correct location of a tone
Finally, Music training is formalized training in conceptual musicianship.

In order to understand this study, you must be familiar with AP. AP listeners are able to name, accurately and quickly, the musical pitch of isolated tones presented in the absence of musical pitch. Proportions of AP possessors supposed range from 1:1,500 to 1:10,000. The percentage of AP possessors among musicians ranges from 3.4% to 15%. No one is certain how people acquire AP, or if it can truly be acquired. However, it could be the result of early musical training. Previous studies have shown that virtually all tested AP possessors had music training by the age of six. Those people who are considered to be “self-reported” AP possessors had all commences earlier music training, meaning they had begun music training as early as ages three to five.

The purpose of this report is to attempt to investigate the learning process of AP. The researchers involved took a cross-section of children aged 4-10 on their pitch naming ability. Because it is a cross-sectional not longitudinal test, the factor of training could not be manipulated for the purpose of the experiment. Also, the effects of the training couldn’t be evaluated.

The musical training of these children was in Tokyo, Japan. The aim of this school was to get children to develop the capability to express themselves by music, not to train professional musicians. The children at the school took a two-year primary course where they had a one-hour weekly lesson, fundamental music skills, and ear training. In this course the teaching method highlighted listening, singing, and playing piano. Children first learn the notes, C4, D4, E4, F4, G4, and C3. Then the children sing with lyrics, sing on solfege, play on piano, and play on piano while singing in solfege in order to remember the pitches. After the children have mastered the white keys, they move onto black ones. When the children have finished the Primary Course, they are able to continue onto the Advances Course, which is just an extension of the basic and applied music skills. Here, the children get to play, compose, arrange, and improvise.

The Method of this test was relatively simple. There were 104 participants (children) aging from 4-10. They were all tested after three months into the school year. The test itself contained a grand piano and an electric piano. The tester would play thirty-six chromatic pitches over a series of three octaves in random order. More than a perfect fifth always separated the successive tones. There were never any octaves so the student couldn’t use relative pitch. The testers wet to every precaution to make the children feel at ease. The students aged 4 were not tested with an electric organ, and all students were given introductory sessions before they were tested.

The primary focus of the results was on the piano tones because not all students were tested with organ. There was a general tendency for accuracy to increase with age, but the accuracy for children ages 5 and 6 was still widely distributed. Also, there was a distinct difference between the fluidity of the children’s knowledge of white keys versus black keys. Most ten year olds knew all of the white keynotes, but still struggled with the black keys. Also, the accuracy with the organ was lower with the organ, particularly with children seven and up. However, all response times remained quick, much to the surprise of the testers.

There are many factors that come into the validity of this test. First of all, all of the children tested came out with varying degrees of absolute pitch. Whether or not they actually had it is debatable. All children were volunteers whose parents responded to a solicitation, so it wasn’t really random selection. Children from the Advanced Course were chosen because of their progress in piano skills, whereas the students from the Primary Course were unselected. Depending on the child, there could be varying degrees of motivation to do well on the test. One could argue that there are notable outcomes if you compare this data with data from a school that isn’t focused on music, but that has yet to be done. This test doesn’t really address the difference of timbres within the experiment or how it affected the performance of the children. Also, it could have been possible for some of the children to use relative pitch, though it would have been difficult.

I would like to end my presentation with some questions that remain after this test was finished. What about standard behavioral physical development in children? Should timbres be introduced at an earlier age? Are the differences in comprehension of white and black keys due to training? Is it actually more difficult for a child to understand black keys? Is this confusion due to a misunderstanding of musical concept? Did they test the Eguchi method for AP training where the children identify chords first, instead of pitches causing them to focus on whole tonal characteristics? Does AP really matter to musicians? Does it interfere with the development of relative pitch?

Thank you for listening.

Sunday, December 03, 2006

Presentation Script

Absolute pitch, also known as perfect pitch, is defined by W.D. Ward and E. M. Burns in their article “Absolute Pitch” as the ability to attach labels to isolated auditory stimuli on the base of pitch alone, or in normal people speak, the ability to identify a note by name with no reference note or produce a correct pitch without reference. There has been much debate on whether or not absolute pitch is genetic or can actually be taught.
This experiment, “Learning Absolute Pitch by Children: A Cross-Sectional Study” was conducted by Ken’ichi Miyazaki and Yoko Ogawa. The experiment took place in a private music school in Tokyo. Children are enrolled at the age of four and being to play piano and gain a sense of musical knowledge through activities using fixed-Do in order to emphasize the base of C4, and are shortly after introduced to other pitches, with a focus on C4, D4 and E4.
In the primary course students learn pitches and pitch names through singing, memorizing, and playing songs on the piano. In the advanced courses, which includes the ages of 6 to 10, students extend their music opportunities to play, compose, arrange, and improvise music. For the experiment 104 children were used including 13-four year olds, 18-five year olds, 13-six year olds, 14-seven year olds, 26-eight year olds, 14-nine year olds, and 6-ten year olds.
The test was held approximately three months after the beginning of the year. Another element of the experiment was the use of different timbres. Both a Yamaha Grand Piano and a Yamaha electric organ ( generating string sounds) were used. The test tones included 36 chromatic pitches over three octaves, however, the participants were not asked to identify pitch class, such as C5, but simply the general name, such as A or B. The pitches were given in a random order and separated by at least 7 semitones (or a Perfect 5) each time in order to discourage the use of relative pitch. Relative pitch is the use of a given note to generate or determine a second note. As was stated earlier two timbres were used however, it was decided against the use of the organ for the 4 year old age group as it seemed they lost interest in the activity very quickly, as most of you are probably losing interest with this presentation already.
The participants responded to the given tone using the solfege fixed-Do system which they had been taught through the school. Fixed-Do means that in every key C is Do and the other pitches are Do-sharp, Re, and so on. There was no feedback give as to whether or not a response was correct but instead constant support and encouragement was given to the participants to keep them motivated through out the experiment. Each participant was tested alone and a video camera was used in order to deteremine response time at a later point.
The results are presented as a series of graphs that I will pass around. The first graphs focus on the responses to the grand piano because not all of the students were tested on the electronic organ. As can be seen in the graphs the tendency is for the percentage correct to increase with the age, however, after age seven it can can also be seen that there is little more improvement. The first graph shows how even though the general line is increasing there are still a few students who were achieving below the overall trend. Each individual is represented by a black dot. In the second graph it shows two lines the first being the white key pitches and the second the black key pitches. As you can see the white key pitches were much more easily recognized then the black key pitches. Again, there was a large increase between in the ages of 4 to 6 and a plateau after the age of 7. The other graphs that I will pass around show the answers given to each given pitch for each age group. At first glance they seem rather confusing so if you have any questions please ask. Figures 4 and 5, or the last two graphs, yes we are nearing the end, show the responses to the organ pitches. These responses are lower and have been rationalized as such because they are not the primary instrument on which the participants studied. Children who had had other training on the electric organ scored much higher.
In general this study did little to prove anything, it simply added more research for an even larger study. This study simply proves that the critical period for the possible development of absolute pitch ends at the age of seven. The study does not settle the issue of etiology of absolute pitch ( etiology is the study of the cause of something) but it can be argued that the data may be used as evidence of the learning process of absolute pitch being effected by music training in childhood.

Musical Rhythm, Linguistic Rhythm, and Human Evolution

This article is called “Musical Rhythm, Linguistic Rhythm, and Human Evolution”. It was written by Aniruddh D. Patel of The Neurosciences Institute
The debate in this study is over the evolutionary status of music – whether evolution has shaped humans to be musical, or whether humans adapt cognitive skills. The journal breaks down music * cognition into four parts as shown here.
Recently, the idea that human minds have been shaped by natural selection for music, first proposed by Darwin, has become widely accepted. There are skeptics, however, who believe music is an “enjoyable mental technology built from preexisting cognitive skills”. The debate can be resolved by determining whether there are fundamental aspects of music cognition which are * innate, that is being born with it, and cannot be explained as being part of cognitive abilities that have been adapted. The author concludes that as of now, there is no reason to reject the idea that * human minds have NOT been specifically shaped by natural selection for music.*
Musical rhythm is similar to speech rhythm, since they both have nice rhythmic organization. They both use pitch movements and durational lengthening, and they both start early in life. Studies have shown that the two use similar brain substrates. This is good evidence that musical rhythm is in fact an offshoot of linguistic rhythm. Musical beats occur in the context of a meter, in which some beats are stronger than others. Interestingly enough, speech is also metrically. based on stress or prominence. This suggests that the tendency to organize rhythmic sequences may originate in language.*
Beat perception and synchronization, or BPS, is a part of rhythm unique to music. It cannot be explained as a byproduct of speech rhythm. The key questions about BPS are about its innateness – its domain specificity, and its human specificity.*
Infants don’t synchronize their movements to a musical beat. This doesn’t mean innateness is false, because infants do not speak. One way to address the innateness of BPS is to look at developmental studies, in order to explore whether the brain seems prepared to acquire this ability. As for now, we don’t have enough information about BPS, including how early one can synchronize to a beat and who can attain this ability. More research is to come…*
One way to study domain-specificity of BPS is to see if brain damage that disrupts it also disrupts other nonmusical cognitive abilities. The neuropsychological literature has descriptions of people with musical rhythmic disturbance after brain damage. It also has findings that rhythmic abilities can be selectively disrupted, leaving pitch processing skills intact. Again… this topic needs more research.*
Animals do not naturally produce music, so if an animal can acquire this ability, it would mean that the ability is not part of an adaptation for music. In all the years of research and animal training, there has not been a single report of an animal being trained to tap, peck, or move in beat. Could an animal learn BPS? If so, this would mean that natural selection is not necessary for BPS.
So there is the question of which animals to study. The obvious answer would be chimps or bonobos, since they are the most closely related to humans. Also, chimps and bonobos have short bouts or rhythmic “drumming” as part of display or play behavior. So we know they are capable of making rhythmic movements on a scale of their own. Despite this, there is still question to whether or not apes are capable of BPS because of brain circuits that are involved in beat perception and motor control. * In humans, rhythms that have a regular beat are associated with the basal ganglia structure in the brain. This structure is also used for motor control and sequencing. But if just this structure were fully in charge of these things, you would expect that chimps, and other species such as rodents, would be capable of BPS. So, because of this, we can conclude that its not just one simple brain function. * This is because BPS involves a special relationship between auditory temporal intervals and patterned movement. This means that somehow or another, human evolution modified the basal ganglia in away that makes for tight coupling between auditory input and motor output.
One way this evolutionary force could have occurred is in vocal learning. This means that you learn to speak or make noise by hearing. This is common to humans, since every child learns to speak by learning. This is only common to a few animals, such as songbirds and parrots. So, humans are unique among primates in having complex vocal learning.
Neurobiological research on birds shows that vocal learning is associated with modifications to the basal ganglia, which is key in auditory input and motor output. So, we can assume that the basal ganglia in humans have also been modified by natural selection for vocal learning.
*So basically, a testable hypothesis would be that…
Having the neural circuitry for complex vocal learning is necessary for the ability to synchronize with an auditory beat. *
This hypothesis pretty much says that if you try to teach nonhuman primates, such as the chimp, to synchronize to a beat, it’s probably not going to work. But, it also says that if it doesn’t work on primates, it would still be premature to conclude that BPS is unique to humans.

The Origins of Music: Theories and their Flaws

In the last decade, the study of music evolution has significantly increased. Ian Cross analyzes two papers on this subject, and points out how they could be stronger in certain areas.

The first paper, by Justus & Hustler, indicates that the capability to learn music is adaptive, and that inherent music talent is the basis for the exploration and reconceptualization of music. The other paper, by McDermott & Hauser is very broad-ranging, and gives a detailed check-list of what one would need to give an evolutionary view of music. While writing their paper, McDermott & Hauser referred to/were influenced by developmental & ethological essays.

Both papers seem to misinterpret the “Neanderthal Flute”, a bone that is mythically believed to be the first musical instrument. McDermott & Hauser suggest that the earliest preserved instruments date back to 6000 BC. This date is to late to be plausible. Justus & Hustler say the “Neanderthal Flute” was made by humans, when in fact, it is believed to be a product of an animals chewing. If it were to be a musical instrument, the date at which is was made is too early a period for music to have started.

Ian Cross also notes that both papers lack specificity when it comes to defining music. McDermott & Hauser state “… a definition of music is not particularly important at this stage.” Cross concludes that their lack of specificity limits their arguments greatly.

McDermott & Hauser end their paper by claiming that music lacks referential precision, because it expresses emotion and is “commonly used to produce enjoyment.” Cross believes that music must be characterized as fully as possible. Only then can you understand how music relates to other aspects of human life, and propose theories on the evolutionary roots of human musicality.

Uses of Music in Everday Life

Many people believe that due to the development of recorded music and mass media, the presence of music has become a part of our everyday lives. There have been many studies that examine just how we experience music from day to day. This one focuses on the five W’s- who, what, when, where, and why.

To answer these questions, the researches found 346 volunteers. The average age of the volunteers was 25.96 years old, but some aged anywhere from 13 to 78 years old. Each participant was asked to complete one approximately 25 minute questionnaire a day via text messaging, and 96.72% of the questionnaires were successfully completed. The participants were very diverse in both ethnic background and occupation, as well as their musical training and experience.

There were six parts to each questionnaire. First, the volunteers were asked if they were either currently listening to music, or if they had at least heard some type of music since the previous survey. If they were not or had not, they did not need to complete the rest of the questions. If they had a music experience to report, they were then asked who they were with during the experience. Next were the questions of what type of styles they had heard, if they were able to choose this music, and if they liked it. The fourth part inquired as to where they had heard the music. The last two parts were split amongst those who had been able to choose what music they experience and those who had not. The group that had a choice was asked why they chose what they did, and the group that had no choice was asked what effects the piece had on them. In both cases, they were given answers to choose from based on previous free response answers to the questions by psychology undergrad.

(Chart 1) After 14 days the study ended, and on 38.6% of the occasions, people could hear music. 60.8% of the times, people could not hear music, and there was no response for .6% of the surveys. (Chart 2) Of the times that people could not hear music, the participants indicated that they had heard some since the last survey was completed 48.6% of the time. For 48.5% of the occasions they had heard no music since the previous survey, and 2.9% of replies contained no response. Overall, this means that the participants had a very high exposure to music.

(Chart 3) Again, the main focus of this study was the 5 W’s, and I’ll start with Who?. Only slightly more than a quarter of the musical experiences were heard by the participant alone. 18.4% of the time, the participant was with friends, 7%, with only a spouse or partner, 8.4%, with family members, 5.8%, with colleagues, 3.2%, with a boyfriend or girlfriend, 1.9%, with strangers, and .6% with someone who was not any of the choices on the list. Obviously, more of the listenings were experience by more than just the participant, which interestingly differs from results of previous studies.

(Chart 4) The next question was what music the participants heard. There were 14 choices to pick from. The most popular answer was Chart pop, heard 38% of the time, followed by R&B/Soul at 8.4% and Dance at 5.3%. Every other choice was picked at least a few times, but no more than 5% of the time. All of the percentages can be seen on chart 4. The results of this part of the survey greatly reflect record sales. For instance, the most heard- chart pop- sells very well, while classical music, 3% of the experiences, does not sell so well.

(Chart 5) The participants were also asked when they could hear the music. The researches made sure to account for the fact that the time of day the messages were sent (which varied day to day) would reflect the answers and used a special formula to ensure the results would not be greatly affected. The responses were split into one hour segments of the day, such as 3-3:59 AM, or 7-7:59 PM. The results are calculated by the times music could be heard compared to the number of people that responded within that hour. The most often experiences took place between 10:00 and 10:59 PM. For more general understanding purposed, the chart splits the responses into the morning and afternoon (8-4:59), and the evening (5-11), including weekdays only. Since many participants were at work for the 8-4:59 time slot, these results are also split between those who could choose and not choose what they listened to. For the people that could choose, 63.9% of the experiences were in the earlier half, while 63.7% were in the later half. For those that could not choose, 36.1% of the responses showed an earlier listening, while 36.3% showed a later listening. (Chart 6) They also split the results into weekends and weekdays. For those that could choose 63.3% of the occurrences took place on weekdays, compared to 66.2% on weekends. Those that could not choose reported that 36.7% of their listening time was on weekdays, compared to 33.8% on weekends. These results greatly contradict thoughts that most music listening happens during leisure time instead of at the workplace. The difference is not significantly large.

(Chart 7) Another section of the survey asked where the volunteers were while they experienced the music. Only half of the experiences took place within the home, while almost 1/5 took place in public places. Also, the two choices that were geared toward people choosing to listen to music, being at home and listening to it on purpose or going to a concert, only made up slightly more than 1/10 of the experiences. These results clearly show that the technological developments in recorded music have a great effect on people’s exposure to music.

(Chart 8) The final part of the survey asked those participants who had chosen to listen to the music they had why they chose to do so. The most common answers were that they either enjoyed the music or it helped to pass the time, while the least common answers had to do with feelings or thought-things like bringing back memories or stimulating an emotion or even just to learn more about the music. (Chart 9) Those who had not chosen to listen to the music were in turn asked what effects the music had on them. While 31.6% of the experiences created the right atmosphere for the situation, and 28.7% of the time the listener enjoyed it, almost 15% of the music annoyed the listener.

So what do these results mean? Almost 70% of the answers showed that the
volunteers were exposed to music at some point in their day. Around 3/4 of the time, the participants were not alone, which means that music plays a large part in social activities. Furthermore, the data of what people are listening to is definitely consistent as to what recorded music people are buying, which shows that more exposure to a certain genre of music could definitely influence a higher sale of that genre. The study also showed that many people are not choosing to listen to the music that they hear, and that most of the instances are taking place at work or in public places, where the mass media has a very large presence. So in a nutshell, this study shows that the increase of mass media and developments in technology do, in fact, have a very large influence on how people are exposed to music every day.

Monday, November 27, 2006

what article is everyone talking about thats due tomorrow!?!?

clue me in, and everyone else that keeps i.m.ong me asking about it.

Sunday, November 19, 2006

Conscious Voluntary Movement Excercise

Created by the fouders of the Pesso Boyden System Psychomotor psychotherapy, CVME is an excercise that helps one maximize their awareness of their bodily control, energy expenditure, and of the placement in space of the portions of the body. In order to teach, it is broken up into four stages: decision-making, planning, implementation, and verification. It is a standing-up excercise that requires first the arm to move back and forward slowly, then other parts of the body, depending upon their field of performance (music, dance). The goal of this technique is to make one completely aware of their movements and thus provide for a perfect performance, as there are no more nervous movements. This technique, so to speak, eliminates nervousness.

This investigation set out to prove whether fourth and fifth garders could audibly recognize the difference between blues melodies and blues improvisation. The students were split into three equally sized groups. The first group memorized and sang a blues melody, the second group was taught how to play a blued melody on the recorder, and the third group was given very little instruction about playing or singing any blues melody. The groups that were taught melodies were able to discern between improv and melody at a much higher rate than the third group.

"Playing without Pain: Strategies for the Developing Instrumentalist"

This articles deals with the realization that not just elder musicians end up with injuries, but that younger musicians are just as susceptible. It states that there are three important factors, proper diagnosis, treatment and prevention. The six focuses that the article looks at are causes, types of difficulties, symptoms, diagnosis, treatment and prevention. The causes of must young musicians injuries are overuse, misuse, and hypermobility. Of course the primary symptom is pain, and the next step is acknowledging that pain means there is something wrong. One suggestion was to take your musical instrument with you when you go to the doctor to be diagnosed. The primary treatment for any injury is rest. The article is mostly informative because in order to prevent the injuries students must know ways in which to do so.



"Playing without Pain: Strategies ofr the Developing Instrumentalist"
By William J. Dawson
Music Educators Journal-November 2006
pg. 36-41

"Taking Aim at Good Intonation"

"Taking Aim at Good Intonation" International Society of Bassists Volume 30, Number 1 - p. 25

This article looks at the steps bassists should take to have good intonation. The ability to play in tune depends on three things: an understanding of the harmonic series and temperaments, ear training, and ability to apply these skills in both practice and performance. The article explains how the intervals are all derived by mathematical ratios, and goes into detail about harmonics and temperaments on the bass using specific examples. It states what a bass player needs to have a good ear and good tone, and includes and exercise to practice to help with both. The last section outlines what the bass player's job is while playing in a group, specifiacally and orchestra, and tips to help with many aspects of playing bass.

"Teaching Music Traders a Lesson"

The Berklee College of Music is known as one of the best and most prestigious contemporary music conservatories in the world. The school hosts a wealth of knowledge ranging from lessons in the traditional jazz technique to the freedom of new, innovative composition. However, Berklee does not wish to hold this abundance of information hostage. In fact, Berklee is encouraging it's students to share lessons, music files, songs, and other musical genius online. This article explores the pros and cons of sharing collegiate information online, and how this new technology will affect music students of the future. In "Teaching Music Traders a Lesson," the author challenges her audience to enter a world where the digital age and the historic age of music collaborate to make a place where it is easy, and accepted, for students to learn through downloading and surfing the web.

Wired News:"Teaching Music Traders a Lesson" by Katie Dean

Saturday, November 18, 2006

The Magic of Rhythm Instruments

Many teachers struggle with trying to raise Pre-school and Kindergarten age children's musical awareness and expressiveness. This article examines the use of rhythm instruments, such as rhythm sticks, shakers sand blocks, and bells, to do this, instead of singing. While the process of singing is very complex for young children in that they must remember the words and the notes, using rhythm instruments can be made fun and simple by using a variety of techniques. Some of these techniques are explained, such as playing along with familiar tunes with different lyrics ("This is the Way We Play Our Sticks" to the tune of "Here We Go Round the Mulberry Bush"), playing games where the children repeat the teacher, telling animal stories where different instruments symbolize different animals (sand blocks hit on the floor become dinosaur footsteps), and pretending the instruments are everyday items (the tambourine becomes a steering wheel). All of these processes are exciting for the children, while at the same time they will increase the children's awareness and expressiveness of music.

This article is from the October 2006 edition of Teaching Music

Monday, November 13, 2006

The Lady with The Hammer


Unusually simple piano scores scatter the studio floor of a reclusive composer in St. Petersburg. Galina Ustwolskaja does not travel and will not take interviews. Dubbed “The Lady with the Hammer” by music critics, blocks of polyphonic sound leap off the pages of her music. Bleak melodies are interrupted by a machine burst in the upper register, embellishing the typical socialist canvas of Soviet composition.
Ustwolskaja’s determination to create music uninfluenced by any other composer or genre made her unsuited to a career in the Soviet music business during the Stalinist era. Regardless, Ustwolskaja had to earn a living and came to an agreement with the state. This meant pumping out charming pieces in the Socialist Realist tradition to please the Soviet State. Her typical music, characterized by driving polyphony, severe dynamics (ffffff and ppppp), the absence of bar lines, and unusual instrumentation (Her Composition No. 1. was written for Piano, Piccolo, and Bass Tuba), was a luxury she mostly kept to herself for personal composition.
Born June 17, 1919, in Petrograd (now St. Petersburg), Uswolskaja began compositional studies at Leningrad Conservatory with Dmitri Shostakovich in 1937 and continued to study with him for ten years. Uswolskaja can be considered the only one of Shostakovich’s students who was able to escape the gravitational pull of his composing planet and create her own compositional style with, according to Uswolskaja, absolutely no influence taken from other composers: “there is no link whatsoever between my music and that of any other composer,”

Even Shostakovich felt he failed to influence her; rather, he felt influenced by Galina. A man unlikely to compliment his students, Shostakovich supported her against the opposition of his colleagues in the Union of Soviet Composers and said of her, ‘I am convinced that the music of G. I. Uswolskaja will achieve worldwide renown, to be valued by all who perceive truth in music to be of paramount importance.” In fact, Shostakovich presented his scores to Ustvolskaya for her approval and attached great value to her comments.
. The scores of Shostakovich’s music for the film Hamlet were similar to Ustwolskaja’s music in mood and method. Use of large percussion blows and whip crack chords with militant simplicity, “almost puritanical in its distrust of anything colorful or soft-edged” The New Shostakovich (1990), expressed a similar experience of oppression under Communism. Otherwise, little connection has been found between Ustwolskaja and Shostakovich’s music.
A personal connection between the two, however, is evident. Rostropovich, a fellow composer who was friends with both of them, declared Uswolskaja and Shostakovich’s relationship as “tender.” In Shostakovich: A Life Remembered, Elizabeth Wilson revealed their union as an “open secret.” Accounts of their relationship are in definite existent, but details are uncertain. The exact how and when of their relationship is unknown. Nevertheless, calling Uswolskaja his "musical conscience” Shostakovich was obviously struck by the young woman. It seems the relationship between the two was short and intense, since Shostakovich married another student soon after accounts of his affair with Uswolskaja were taken. It is known she broke up with the fellow composer in 1956, when she was 37 years old and the only contribution of the affair given by Uswolskaja herself is bitter and abrupt: “One thing remains as clear as day: a seemingly eminent figure such as Shostakovich, to me, is not eminent at all, on the contrary he burdened my life and killed my best feelings."